Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Hum Brain Mapp. 2022 Jul; 43(10): 3153–3163.

Published online 2022 Mar 22. doi:10.1002/hbm.25840

PMCID: PMC9189080

PMID: 35315958

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Both monetary loss and pain have been studied for decades, but evidence supporting the relationship between them is still lacking. We conducted a meta‐analysis to explore the overlapping brain regions between monetary loss and pain, including physical pain and social pain. Regardless of the type of pain experienced, activation of the anterior insula was a shared neural representation of monetary loss and pain. The network representation pattern of monetary loss was more similar to that of social pain than that of physical pain. In conclusion, our research provided evidence of the common neural correlates of monetary loss and pain.

Keywords: meta‐analysis, monetary loss, physical pain, social pain

Regardless of the type of pain experienced, activation of the anterior insula was a shared neural representation of monetary loss and pain. The network representation pattern of monetary loss was more similar to that of social pain than that of physical pain.

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (3)

1. INTRODUCTION

Money is considered to have multiple psychological meanings for its functions of exchanging goods and services, and motivating others (Lea & Webley,2006). For example, money can elicit both positive and negative emotions (Tang,1992), inspire individuals’ internal motivations (Lea & Webley,2006), reduce the harm caused by low self‐esteem (Zhang,2009), and transform the norms of interpersonal relationships (Vohs, Mead, & Goode,2008; Zaleskiewicz & Gasiorowska,2016). Monetary loss refers to the loss of money (Kamarajan et al.,2009), which can alter individuals’ behavior and mental states. Individuals report more negative emotions after monetary loss relative to monetary gains (Jang, Lin, & Lustig,2020). Monetary loss can also act as punishment to improve individuals’ behavioral performance (Li, Cox, Or, & Blandford,2016).

People often viewed the experience of monetary loss as undesirable and disgusting. For example, people usually described the experience of monetary loss with words that described physically painful experiences such as using the word “headache” to depict the psychological state of receiving a costly bill (Xu, Zhou, Ye, & Zhou,2015). Moreover, some researchers have found that monetary loss directly affects individuals’ sensitivity to pain, since monetary loss might amplify the painful feelings of nociceptive stimuli or social exclusion (Zhou, Vohs, & Baumeister,2009).

All these results suggested a psychological overlap between monetary loss and pain including physical pain and social pain, two typical types of pain, which have been widely investigated in pain‐related research (Merskey,1986). Physical pain refers to the displeasing sensory and affective experience caused by real or potential body injuries (Merskey,1986), whereas social pain refers to the painful experience of social harm when social relationships are threatened, damaged or lost, such as social exclusion (Eisenberger & Lieberman,2004; Rotge et al.,2015). Despite the linguistic overlap and mutual relationship between monetary loss and pain, one problem we remained unclear about was whether the processing of monetary loss and pain engaged overlapping neural bases.

Previous studies have detected neural activity related to monetary loss. The receipt of monetary loss activates a broad range of brain regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal cortex, insula, cuneus, lingual gyrus, and parahippocampal gyrus (García, Filbey, Dunlop, & Myers,2013; Kocsel et al.,2017, 2019; Lauwereyns, Bjork, Smith, Chen, & Hommer,2010; Luo et al.,2019; Ubl et al.,2015; Yan et al.,2016; Yu, Duan, Wu, & Luo,2021; Yu, Wu, Huang, & Luo,2020). Moreover, a meta‐analysis of monetary loss found that the outcome of monetary loss consistently activated the striatum and anterior cingulate gyri (Dugré, Dumais, Bitar, & Potvin,2018).

The neurocognitive network underlying physical pain and social pain were similar to those underlying monetary loss. The processing of physical pain involves a specific brain network referred to as the “pain matrix,” including the sensory‐discriminative system and cognitive‐affective system (Wager et al.,2013; Wiech,2016). The sensory‐discriminative system is responsible for recognizing the intensity, location, and quality of nociceptive stimuli and this system includes the lateral thalamus, posterior insula, and primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. The cognitive‐affective system is responsible for processing the psychological characteristics of physical pain, and this system includes the anterior insula (AI) and ACC. Regarding social pain, previous studies have found that social pain activates the insula, ACC, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Cristofori, Harquel, Isnard, Mauguiere, & Sirigu,2015; Eisenberger & Lieberman,2004; Luo et al.,2015; Luo et al.,2015; Luo, Shi, Yang, Wang, & Han,2014). Moreover, the activity of the ACC and AI positively correlate with self‐reported distress (DeWall et al.,2010; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams,2003; Luo et al.,2018; Luo, Yu, & Han,2016; Luo & Zhang,2018a, 2018b; Luo, Zhu, Kong, & Xu,2017; Masten et al.,2009). Therefore, monetary loss and pain, including physical pain and social pain, may recruit a common neural substrate, such as insula and ACC.

In this study, we first conducted a neuroimaging meta‐analysis to investigate the overlapping neural bases of monetary loss and pain, including physical pain and social pain, with the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) technique (Eickhoff et al.,2009; Eickhoff, Bzdok, Laird, Kurth, & Fox,2012). Furthermore, if the processing of monetary loss and pain, including social pain and physical pain, engaged overlapping brain regions, we wanted to further explore which type of pain exhibited a more similar neural representation to monetary loss by representational similarity analysis (RSA; Huang, Yu, Long, Huang, & Luo,2022; Peng & Luo,2021; Yuan, Long, Huang, Huang, & Luo,2022).

2. METHODS

2.1. Literature search

For each domain, we searched relevant articles in Google Scholar on December 27, 2019, and supplemented the included literature by searching articles in PubMed on November 16, 2021. For monetary loss, we used the terms fMRI AND (“monetary loss” OR “lose money” OR “lost money” OR “loses money” OR “money loss” OR “financial loss”) and identified 4012 studies. For physical pain, we used the terms fMRI AND pain AND (“electric stimuli” OR “thermal stimuli” OR “mechanical stimuli” OR “pressure stimuli”) AND participants and identified 4,514 studies. For social pain, we used the terms fMRI AND pain AND (“social pain” OR “social rejection” OR “social exclusion” OR “social ostracism”) and identified 8,103 studies.

2.2. Sample selection

We screened all the studies according to the following criteria (Figure1): (a) only FMRI studies; (b) only studies that contained whole‐brain activation coordinates; (c) only studies whose activation coordinates were represented in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space or Talairach (TAL) space; (d) only studies involving normal participants; (e) only original English studies published in a peer‐reviewed journal; (f) only studies without priming or drug administration, since the drug or priming may affect the sensitivity of loss and pain (Bradley et al.,2020; Urtado Silva, Galhardoni, Ciampi de Andrade, & Brito,2019); and (e) other special criteria. For monetary loss, only studies containing the outcome phase of monetary loss were included, since loss anticipation and outcome may activate distinct brain regions (Dugré et al.,2018). For physical pain, only studies including experimental pain rather than chronic pain were included. Additionally, we explored the brain areas consistently activated across different nociceptive stimuli, and we did not restrict the type of painful stimuli. For social pain, we only included studies involving a direct experience of social pain. Three researchers screened all relevant studies and reached a consensus on these inclusion criteria.

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (4)

The flowchart of the sample selection process

2.3. ALE procedure

We conducted the meta‐analysis with the ALE method using the GingerALE software package (Eickhoff et al.,2009, 2012; Eickhoff, Laird, Fox, Lancaster, & Fox,2016).

2.3.1. Independent meta‐analysis

We first performed an individual meta‐analysis of each domain to examine the most concurrent brain regions in each domain. Only coordinates with positive activations were included in our analyses. Additionally, we excluded those activation coordinates detected by a more liberal threshold than the threshold used for the rest of the brain. We first used GingerALE software to convert all the TAL coordinates into MNI coordinates (Eickhoff et al.,2009). Then, we used a three‐dimensional Gaussian probability distribution to represent the spatial uncertainty of each activation coordinate, in which the value of each voxel was the probability that the activation coordinate was located in this voxel. In each study, for each voxel, we calculated the probability that at least one activation coordinate was located in this voxel, which was called the model activation (MA) value representing the probability of activation of this voxel in this study. Then, we obtained an MA map for each study. Through the integration of all the MA maps, we calculated the ALE value for each voxel, representing the probability that at least one activation coordinate was located in this voxel when considering all relevant papers. Finally, we conducted a permutation test to examine whether these MA maps overlapped. We randomly selected an activation coordinate and its corresponding MA value for each MA map and then calculated an ALE value based on these MA values. After repeating these steps several times, we constructed the null distribution based on the obtained ALE values and calculated the p‐value for each voxel. We applied cluster‐level family‐wise error correction and used p < .001 as the cluster‐forming threshold and p < .05 for cluster‐level inference to identify the significantly activated brain regions and correct for false‐positive inflation resulting from multiple comparisons (Eickhoff et al.,2009).

2.3.2. Conjunction and contrast analysis

Before conducting the conjunction and contrast analysis, we first merged all the original activation coordinates for each domain pair (monetary loss–physical pain; monetary loss–social pain) and conducted an individual meta‐analysis based on these pooled coordinates.

We conducted a conjunction analysis by taking the voxelwise minimum ALE value of those two thresholded individual meta‐analysis‐derived networks to determine the overlapping brain regions for each domain pair.

We also conducted a contrast analysis for each domain pair to determine the distinct brain regions. For each domain pair, we first pooled all the original activation coordinates of those two meta‐analysis networks and randomly split them into two groups (Eickhoff et al.,2011). We calculated the voxelwise ALE values for each group and computed the difference in the ALE values between those two groups for each voxel. After iterating these steps 10,000 times, we produced a null distribution and calculated the p‐value for each voxel (Laird et al.,2005). Finally, we applied an uncorrected threshold of p < .01 with a minimum cluster size of 200 mm3 to correct for false‐positive inflation resulting from multiple comparisons. The results of contrast analyses are provided in the Supporting Information.

2.4. Similarity analysis

To further analyze the similarity in neural representations between each domain pair (monetary loss–physical pain; monetary–social pain), according to Schurz et al.(2021), we conducted similarity analyses based on the meta‐analytical results at a less conservative statistical threshold of p < .005 uncorrected with a minimum cluster size of 200 mm3.

For each meta‐analysis‐derived network, we categorized all the significantly activated voxels based on the 17‐network parcellation template reported by Yeo et al.(2011). We calculated the percentage of the significant voxels located in each subnetwork among all the voxels significantly activated by that domain, which consisted of the distribution vector. For each network pair, the Spearman correlation coefficient and the Euclidean distance between their corresponding distribution vectors were calculated as indices of their similarity in neural representations.

Additionally, we conducted a, RSA based on the representational similarity between those 17 subnetworks within each meta‐analysis‐derived network. For each domain, we used the following formula to calculate the similarity of each pairwise subnetwork:

Smn=1|PmPn|

(1)

where Smn represents the similarity of subnetworks m and n, and Pm and Pn represent the percentages of the significant voxels located in subnetworks m and n among all the voxels significantly activated by that domain, respectively. Then, we acquired a similarity matrix for each meta‐analysis‐derived network, in which the value of each cell was defined as the similarity of each corresponding pairwise subnetwork. For each domain pair, we calculated the Spearman correlation coefficient between their corresponding similarity matrices and tested it using the Mantel permutation test (Mantel,1967), which was a one‐tailed test. The null hypothesis was that the Spearman correlation coefficient between two similarity matrices was ≤0. We randomly shuffled the rows and columns of one similarity matrix to construct a new similarity matrix and then calculated the Spearman correlation coefficient between this new similarity matrix and the remaining similarity matrix. After repeating these steps many times, we constructed a null distribution and calculated a p‐value for the observed Spearman correlation coefficient.

2.5. Meta‐analytical functional decoding

We decoded the functional characteristics of the meta‐analysis network of each domain using the Neurosynth Image Decoder. This platform calculates the similarity between any meta‐analysis network and other meta‐analytical maps related to certain terms by computing Pearson's correlation coefficients across all voxels (Bellucci, Molter, & Park,2019). For each domain, we selected four relevant meta‐analytical activation maps (monetary loss domain: stop signal, choices, reward, and error; physical pain domain: somatosensory, sensation, nociceptive, and stimulation; and social pain domain: affective, social, emotional, and mood).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Individual meta‐analysis

For monetary loss, one study was excluded from the final ALE meta‐analysis because it reported no significant activation coordinates. The monetary loss dataset (TableS1) consisted of 469 participants and 20 studies containing 20 experiments and 151 foci. The ALE meta‐analysis of all foci (Figure2a and Table1) revealed four significant clusters, involving the right AI (extending to the inferior frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus), left AI (extending to the inferior frontal gyrus), anterior cingulate, and lingual gyrus (extending to the inferior occipital gyrus).

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (5)

Overview of significant clusters identified in the individual meta‐analyses and conjunction analysis. (a) Meta‐analysis of monetary loss. (b) Meta‐analysis of physical pain. (c) Meta‐analysis of social pain. (d) Conjunction analysis. ALE, activation likelihood estimation; ML‐SP, the conjunction analysis between monetary loss and social pain (shown in green); ML‐PP, the conjunction analysis between monetary loss and physical pain (shown in red)

TABLE 1

Significant clusters revealed by the individual meta‐analyses

ClusterVolume (mm3)xyzRegion
Monetary loss
145283220−12Insula
4822−10Inferior frontal gyrus/insula
5018−24Superior temporal gyrus
21640−3416−12Insula
−4822−16Inferior frontal gyrus/insula
3148064218Anterior cingulate
23640Anterior cingulate
64028Anterior cingulate
41296−16−88−12Lingual gyrus
−12−94−2Inferior occipital gyrus
Physical pain
174085224Precentral gyrus
36812Insula
24144Lentiform nucleus
40182Insula
24696−38−1614Insula
−64−2828Inferior parietal lobule
−58−1818Postcentral gyrus
340002840Anterior cingulate
42030Anterior cingulate
−23236Anterior cingulate
−41250Anterior cingulate
4389614−108Thalamus
14614Caudate
−14−1810Thalamus
−8−44Thalamus
5364058−2022Postcentral gyrus
60−3438Supramarginal gyrus
64−2826Inferior parietal lobule
62328−32166Claustrum
71024−42−4−2Insula
8102438−2018Insula
44−1214Insula
Social pain
12784−4634−14Inferior frontal gyrus
−3624−4Insula
22048−234−16Anterior cingulate
−1246−2Anterior cingulate
31560−14−924Lingual gyrus
41216026−4Anterior cingulate

Note: The MNI coordinates (x, y, z) are provided.

The physical pain dataset (TableS2) included 615 participants and 30 studies containing 32 experiments and 568 foci. ALE meta‐analyses of all foci revealed eight significant clusters (Figure2b and Table1), involving the right AI (extending to the precentral gyrus and lentiform nucleus), left posterior insula (extending to the inferior parietal lobule and postcentral gyrus), anterior cingulate, thalamus (extending to the caudate), postcentral gyrus (extending to the supramarginal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule), right posterior insula, and claustrum.

The social pain dataset (TableS3) comprised 1,201 participants and 32 studies including 34 experiments and 376 foci. ALE meta‐analyses of all foci revealed four significant clusters (Figure2c and Table1), involving the left AI (extending to the inferior frontal gyrus), anterior cingulate gyrus, and lingual gyrus.

3.2. Conjunction analysis

The conjunction analysis between physical pain and monetary loss (Figure2d and Table2) revealed two significant clusters, which were located in the right AI and dorsal anterior cingulate. The conjunction analysis between social pain and monetary loss (Figure2d and Table2) revealed three significant clusters involving the left AI (extending to the inferior frontal gyrus), inferior occipital gyrus, and lingual gyrus.

TABLE 2

Significant clusters revealed by the conjunction analysis

ClusterVolume (mm3)xyzRegion
Monetary loss ∩ Physical pain
12964420−6Insula
4020−6Insula
216043440Dorsal anterior cingulate
Monetary loss ∩ Social pain
1376−12−94−2Inferior occipital gyrus
2120−3624−14Insula/inferior frontal gyrus
−4424−16Insula/inferior frontal gyrus
−4022−14Insula/inferior frontal gyrus
332−14−90−8Lingual gyrus

Note: The MNI coordinates (x, y, z) are provided.

3.3. Similarity analysis

For each meta‐analysis‐derived network, we calculated the percentage of the significant voxels located in each subnetwork among all the voxels significantly activated by that domain (Figure3). The monetary loss meta‐analysis‐derived network was mainly located within the visual peripheral, limbic, and default A network; the physical pain meta‐analysis‐derived network was mainly located within the control A, default C, and default A networks; and the social pain meta‐analysis‐derived network was mainly located within the limbic and visual peripheral networks. The Spearman correlation coefficient between the distribution vectors of monetary loss and social pain was marginally significant (Spearman's ρ = .44, p = .079), while the Spearman correlation coefficient between the distribution vectors of monetary loss and physical pain was insignificant (Spearman's ρ = −.01, p = .959). Consistently, the distribution vector of monetary loss was closer to the distribution vector of social pain than to physical pain (the Euclidean distance between monetary loss and social pain: .19; the Euclidean distance between monetary loss and physical pain: .46).

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (6)

Results of the similarity analysis. (a, b) Line and bar charts of the percentage of the significant voxels located in each subnetwork among all the voxels significantly activated by that domain. ED, Euclidean distance. (c) Representational similarity matrices of three domains. The value of each cell represents the similarity of each pairwise subnetwork. The name of 17 subnetworks: 1—visual peripheral network, 2—visual central network, 3—somatomotor A network, 4—somatomotor B network, 5—dorsal attention A network, 6—dorsal attention B network, 7—ventral attention network, 8—salience network, 9—limbic network, 10—limbic network, 11—control C network, 12—control A network, 13—control B network, 14—default D network, 15—default C network, 16—default A network, and 17—default B network. *p < .05 and **p < .01

The results of the RSA showed a similar pattern (Figure3). Monetary loss shared more similar neural representations with social pain than physical pain (the Spearman correlation coefficient between the similarity matrices of monetary loss and social pain: Spearman's ρ = .51, p = .002; the Spearman correlation coefficient between the similarity matrices of monetary loss and physical pain: Spearman's ρ = .02, p = .378).

All these results suggested that the neural representation of monetary loss was more similar to social pain than to physical pain.

3.4. Meta‐analytical functional decoding

As shown in Figure4, the meta‐analysis network of each domain was more closely related to the functional roles in the corresponding domain relative to the other domains. Moreover, the monetary loss meta‐analysis network was more closely related to the functional roles in the social pain domain than the physical pain domain, confirming the hypothesis that the monetary loss meta‐analysis‐derived network was more similar to the social pain meta‐analysis network than to the physical pain meta‐analysis‐derived network.

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (7)

Results of meta‐analytical functional decoding

4. DISCUSSION

Current research has shown that monetary loss shares common neural bases with pain. We found that monetary loss and pain, whether physical pain or social pain, engaged overlapping neural regions. Although monetary loss and physical pain coactivated the right AI and dorsal anterior cingulate, monetary loss and social pain coactivated the left AI, inferior occipital gyrus, and lingual gyrus. Furthermore, the neural representation of monetary loss was more similar to social pain than to physical pain. All these results provided persuasive evidence of common neural correlates of monetary loss and pain.

Regardless of the type of pain experienced, activation of the AI was a shared neural representation of monetary loss and pain. The AI is a multifunctional brain region that is involved in various cognitive, perceptual, and socio‐affective processes (Clos, Rottschy, Laird, Fox, & Eickhoff,2014; Kurth, Zilles, Fox, Laird, & Eickhoff,2010). In particular, activation of the insula plays an important role in affective processing (Koelsch, Cheung, Jentschke, & Haynes,2021). Therefore, coactivation of the AI might reflect that monetary loss and pain engaged an overlapping neural module of affective processing. Moreover, monetary loss and physical pain coactivated the right AI, whereas monetary loss and social pain coactivated the left AI. This result was consistent with previous findings that the AI was right‐lateralized in connectivity with the postcentral gyrus and superior parietal lobule, which were part of the physical pain network (Kann, Zhang, Manza, Leung, & Li,2016), whereas the left AI was part of memory and socioemotional networks (Clos et al.,2014), and the activation of the left AI was associated with maintaining the feelings of others in working memory (Smith et al.,2017).

The comparison of activation patterns used three algorithms and consistently showed that the monetary loss network was more similar to the social pain network than to the physical pain network. For individuals, money is not only a physical stimulus but also has rich emotional and social meanings to people, since money can arouse positive or negative emotions (Tang,1992; Yu, Huang, Mao, & Luo,2022), elicit individuals’ internal motivation (Lea & Webley,2006), reduce the harm caused by low self‐esteem (Zhang,2009) and change the norms of interpersonal relationships (Vohs et al.,2008; Zaleskiewicz & Gasiorowska,2016). Moreover, this result provided neural evidence of the social resource theory of money, in which money was regarded as a type of social resource, similar to social relationships, which might elicit pain and a sense of security (Zhou et al.,2009; Zhou & Gao,2008).

We admitted that our research had several limitations. For one thing, results of similar analyses may be due to higher similarities in sensory system between monetary loss and social pain compared to physical pain. The reason was that the sensory system of physical pain was the somatomotor system while the sensory system of both monetary loss and social pain was the visual system. However, we excluded this possibility by replicating the similarity analyses without including the somatomotor and visual networks. We got similar results to the previous ones (FiguresS3 and S4), suggesting that the neural representation of monetary loss shared more similarities with that of social pain beyond the level of sensory system. For another thing, our research investigated the shared neural bases underlying monetary loss and pain by exploring whether processing these two events involved overlapping neural regions while ignoring the possibility of the involvement of overlapping functional connectivities. Previous studies have reported that the processing of social stimuli and monetary stimuli recruits overlapping functional connectivities by investigating the relationship between social reward and monetary reward (Gu et al.,2019). However, we remained unclear whether the processing of monetary loss and social pain also involved overlapping functional connectivities. Future studies could use meta‐analytic connectivity modeling to investigate the neural correlates of monetary loss and social pain from the perspective of functional connectivity.

5. CONCLUSIONS

In conclusion, our research revealed the shared neural bases underlying monetary loss and pain. We showed that the processing of monetary loss and pain engaged overlapping neural regions by conducting an ALE neuroimaging meta‐analysis. Regardless of the type of pain experienced, activation of the AI was a shared neural representation of monetary loss and pain. Moreover, the neural representation of monetary loss was more similar to that of social pain than to that of physical pain. Therefore, our research provided persuasive neural evidence for the shared neural bases between monetary loss and pain.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Designed the research: Huixin Tan, Yihan Liu, Xinyu Qiao, and Siyang Luo. Conducted the experiments: Huixin Tan, Yihan Liu, and Xinyu Qiao. Analyzed the data: Huixin Tan, Qin Duan, Yihan Liu, and Xinyu Qiao. Wrote the manuscript: Huixin Tan, Qin Duan, and Siyang Luo. Commented on the manuscript: Huixin Tan, Qin Duan Yihan Liu, Xinyu Qiao, and, Siyang Luo.

ETHICS STATEMENT

All procedures in studies involving human participants were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Department of Psychology of Sun Yat‐sen University Ethics Committee.

Supporting information

AppendixS1: Supporting Information

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Projects 32071081 and 31800916 to Siyang Luo) and the Guangdong Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation (Project 2020A1515010975 to Siyang Luo).

Notes

Tan, H., Duan, Q., Liu, Y., Qiao, X., & Luo, S. (2022). Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain. Human Brain Mapping, 43(10), 3153–3163. 10.1002/hbm.25840 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Funding information Guangdong Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation, Grant/Award Number: 2020A1515010975; National Natural Science Foundation of China, Grant/Award Numbers: 32071081, 31800916

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

All data are available in Tables S1–S3. The thresholded and unthresholded meta‐analysis map can be downloaded from https://neurovault.org/collections/11982/. The software used in our research is publicly accessible: GingerALE 2.3.6 (http://www.brainmap.org/ale/), and R 4.0.4 (https://www.r-project.org/).

REFERENCES

  • Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), 5184. 10.1038/s41467-019-13261-8 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley, E. R., Brustkern, J., De Coster, L., van den Bos, W., McClure, S. M., Seitz, A., & Woolley, J. D. (2020). Victory is its own reward: Oxytocin increases costly competitive behavior in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 50(4), 674–682. 10.1017/S0033291719000552 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Clos, M., Rottschy, C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2014). Comparison of structural covariance with functional connectivity approaches exemplified by an investigation of the left anterior insula. NeuroImage, 99, 269–280. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.030 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cristofori, I., Harquel, S., Isnard, J., Mauguiere, F., & Sirigu, A. (2015). Monetary reward suppresses anterior insula activity during social pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(12), 1668–1676. 10.1093/scan/nsv054 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • DeWall, C. N., MacDonald, G., Webster, G. D., Masten, C. L., Baumeister, R. F., Powell, C., … Eisenberger, N. I. (2010). Acetaminophen reduces social pain. Psychological Science, 21(7), 931–937. 10.1177/0956797610374741 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dugré, J. R., Dumais, A., Bitar, N., & Potvin, S. (2018). Loss anticipation and outcome during the Monetary Incentive Delay Task: A neuroimaging systematic review and meta‐analysis. PeerJ, 6, e4749. 10.7717/peerj.4749 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Kurth, F., & Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis revisited. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2349–2361. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Roski, C., Caspers, S., Zilles, K., & Fox, P. T. (2011). Co‐activation patterns distinguish cortical modules, their connectivity and functional differentiation. NeuroImage, 57(3), 938–949. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.021 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. M., Lancaster, J. L., & Fox, P. T. (2016). Implementation errors in the GingerALE Software: Description and recommendations. Human Brain Mapping, 38(1), 7–11. 10.1002/hbm.23342 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., & Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2907–2926. 10.1002/hbm.20718 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294–300. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290–292. 10.1126/science.1089134 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • García, A. V., Filbey, F. M., Dunlop, J., & Myers, U. S. (2013). Neural effects of positive and negative incentives during Marijuana withdrawal. PLoS One, 8(5), e61470. 10.1371/journal.pone.0061470 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gu, R., Huang, W., Camilleri, J., Xu, P., Wei, P., Eickhoff, S. B., & Feng, C. (2019). Love is analogous to money in human brain: Coordinate‐based and functional connectivity meta‐analyses of social and monetary reward anticipation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 108–128. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.017 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Huang, G., Yu, X., Long, Q., Huang, L., & Luo, S. (2022). The impact of economic freedom on COVID‐19 pandemic control: the moderating role of equality. Globalization and Health, 18, 15. 10.1186/s12992-022-00800-0 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Jang, H., Lin, Z., & Lustig, C. (2020). Losing money and motivation: Effects of loss incentives on motivation and metacognition in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1489. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01489 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kamarajan, C., Porjesz, B., Rangaswamy, M., Tang, Y., Chorlian, D. B., Padmanabhapillai, A., … Begleiter, H. (2009). Brain signatures of monetary loss and gain: Outcome‐related potentials in a single outcome gambling task. Behavioural Brain Research, 197(1), 62–76. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.011 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kann, S., Zhang, S., Manza, P., Leung, H.‐C., & Li, C.‐S. R. (2016). Hemispheric lateralization of resting‐state functional connectivity of the anterior insula: Association with age, gender, and a novelty‐seeking trait. Brain Connectivity, 6(9), 724–734. 10.1089/brain.2016.0443 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kocsel, N., Galambos, A., Szabo, E., Edes, A. E., Magyar, M., Zsombok, T., … Juhasz, G. (2019). Altered neural activity to monetary reward/loss processing in episodic migraine. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 5420. 10.1038/s41598-019-41867-x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kocsel, N., Szabo, E., Galambos, A., Edes, A., Pap, D., Elliott, R., … Kokonyei, G. (2017). Trait rumination influences neural correlates of the anticipation but not the consumption phase of reward processing. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 85. 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00085 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Koelsch, S., Cheung, V. K. M., Jentschke, S., & Haynes, J. D. (2021). Neocortical substrates of feelings evoked with music in the ACC, insula, and somatosensory cortex. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 10119. 10.1038/s41598-021-89405-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kurth, F., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., Laird, A. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta‐analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 519–534. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Laird, A. R., Fox, P. M., Price, C. J., Glahn, D. C., Uecker, A. M., Lancaster, J. L., … Fox, P. T. (2005). ALE meta‐analysis: Controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 155–164. 10.1002/hbm.20136 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lauwereyns, J., Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2010). Adolescents, adults and rewards: Comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS One, 5(7), e11440. 10.1371/journal.pone.0011440 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lea, S. E., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(2), 161–176. 10.1017/S0140525X06009046 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Li, S. Y., Cox, A. L., Or, C., & Blandford, A. (2016). Effects of monetary reward and punishment on information checking behaviour. Applied Ergonomics, 53, 258–266. 10.1016/j.apergo.2015.10.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Li, B., Ma, Y., Zhang, W., Rao, Y., & Han, S. (2015). Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy‐related brain activity. NeuroImage, 110, 22–31. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.042 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Ma, Y., Liu, Y., Wang, C., Li, B., Zhang, W., … Han, S. (2015). Interaction between oxytocin receptor polymorphism and interdependence on human empathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1273–1281. 10.1093/scan/nsv019 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Shi, Z., Yang, X., Wang, X., & Han, S. (2014). Reminders of mortality decrease midcingulate activity in response to others' suffering. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 477–486. 10.1093/scan/nst010 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Wu, B., Fan, X., Zhu, Y., Wu, X., & Han, S. (2019). Thoughts of death affect reward learning by modulating salience network activity. NeuroImage, 202, 116068. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116068 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Yu, D., & Han, S. (2016). Genetic and neural correlates of romantic relationship satisfaction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 337–348. 10.1093/scan/nsv117 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., & Zhang, X. (2018a). Empathy in female submissive BDSM practitioners. Neuropsychologia, 116, 44–58. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.027 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., & Zhang, X. (2018b). Embodiment and humiliation moderation of neural responses to others’ suffering in female submissive BDSM practitioners. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 463. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00463 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Zhong, S., Zhu, Y., Wang, C., Yang, J., Gu, L., … Wu, X. (2018). Brain structural and functional substrates of personal distress in empathy. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 99. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00099 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Luo, S., Zhu, Y., Kong, Q., & Xu, Y. (2017). The oxytocinergic system modulates sad*stic context‐dependent empathy in humans. Scientific Reports, 7, 12463. 10.1038/s41598-017-12671-2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mantel, N. (1967). The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach. Cancer Research, 27(2), 209–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 143–157. 10.1093/scan/nsp007 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Merskey, H. E. (1986). Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Pain, 3, 226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Peng, L., & Luo, S. (2021). Impact of social economic development on personality traits among Chinese college students: A cross‐temporal meta‐analysis, 2001‐2016. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110461. 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110461 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Rotge, J. Y., Lemogne, C., Hinfray, S., Huguet, P., Grynszpan, O., Tartour, E., … Fossati, P. (2015). A meta‐analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(1), 19–27. 10.1093/scan/nsu110 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schurz, M., Radua, J., Tholen, M. G., Maliske, L., Margulies, D. S., Mars, R. B., … Kanske, P. (2021). Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta‐analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 147(3), 293–327. 10.1037/bul0000303 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Alkozei, A., Bao, J., Smith, C., Sanova, A., … Killgore, W. D. (2017). Maintaining the feelings of others in working memory is associated with activation of the left anterior insula and left frontal‐parietal control network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(5), 848–860. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tang, T. L.‐P. (1992). The meaning of money revisited. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(2), 197–202. 10.1002/job.4030130209 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ubl, B., Kuehner, C., Kirsch, P., Ruttorf, M., Diener, C., & Flor, H. (2015). Altered neural reward and loss processing and prediction error signalling in depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(8), 1102–1112. 10.1093/scan/nsu158 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Urtado Silva, J., Galhardoni, R., Ciampi de Andrade, D., & Brito, I. (2019). Effects of intranasal oxytocin on tactile perception. Neuroscience Letters, 698, 64–68. 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.12.032 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2008). Merely activating the concept of money changes personal and interpersonal behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 208–212. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00576.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C.‐W., & Kross, E. (2013). An fMRI‐based neurologic signature of physical pain. New England Journal of Medicine, 368(15), 1388–1397. 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wiech, K. (2016). Deconstructing the sensation of pain: The influence of cognitive processes on pain perception. Science, 354(6312), 584–587. 10.1126/science.aaf8934 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Xu, Q., Zhou, Y., Ye, M., & Zhou, X. (2015). Perceived social support reduces the pain of spending money. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 219–230. 10.1016/j.jcps.2014.11.004 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yan, C., Wang, Y., Su, L., Xu, T., Yin, D.‐Z., Fan, M.‐X., … Chan, R. C. K. (2016). Differential mesolimbic and prefrontal alterations during reward anticipation and consummation in positive and negative schizotypy. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 254, 127–136. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.06.014 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., … Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yu, M., Duan, Q., Wu, X., & Luo, S. (2021). Effect of uncertainty‐dependent residential mobility on loss aversion. PsyCh Journal, 10(5), 777–793. 10.1002/pchj.475 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yu, M., Huang, L., Mao, J., & Luo, S. (2022). Childhood maltreatment, automatic negative thoughts, and resilience: The protective roles of culture and genes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 37(1‐2), 349–370. 10.1177/0886260520912582 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yu, M., Wu, X., Huang, L., & Luo, S. (2020). Residential mobility mindset enhances temporal discounting in the loss framework. Physiology & Behavior, 225, 113107. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113107 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yuan, H., Long, Q., Huang, G., Huang, L., & Luo, S. (2022). Different roles of interpersonal trust and institutional trust in COVID‐19 pandemic control. Social Science & Medicine, 293, 114677. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114677 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zaleskiewicz, T., & Gasiorowska, A. (2016). The Routledge international handbook of consumer psychology (pp. 312–326). New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  • Zhang, L. (2009). An exchange theory of money and self‐esteem in decision making. Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 66–76. 10.1037/a0014225 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zhou, X., & Gao, D.‐G. (2008). Social support and money as pain management mechanisms. Psychological Inquiry, 19(3–4), 127–144. 10.1080/10478400802587679 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). The symbolic power of money: reminders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6), 700–706. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02353.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley-Blackwell

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain (2024)

FAQs

Does losing money truly hurt? The shared neural bases of monetary loss and pain? ›

Regardless of the type of pain experienced, activation of the anterior insula was a shared neural representation of monetary loss and pain. The network representation pattern of monetary loss was more similar to that of social pain than that of physical pain.

Why does losing money hurt so much? ›

People with higher levels of transporters and therefore lower levels of norepinephrine or noradrenaline have what is known as "loss aversion," where they have a more pronounced emotional response to losses compared to gains. Loss aversion can vary widely between people, the researchers explained.

How does money affect the brain? ›

More recently, we've also discovered that low income is associated with physical changes in brain development. For example, children from low-income families tend to develop a smaller hippocampus—a part of the brain that is important for learning and memory.

How does the brain react to pain? ›

Your brain receives and processes these messages, evaluates them, and decides what to do. Your brain sends messages back to your body to react. Depending on the situation, this could mean rapidly triggering the muscles in your body to move you away from whatever is causing your pain.

How long does it take for the brain to register pain? ›

However, because of the low peripheral and spinal conduction velocities, pain-related information from the hand arrives at the human brain not earlier than ∼100 ms after stimulus application.

What is the trauma of losing money? ›

Excessive fear of poverty or financial ruin is a prevalent emotional response associated with financial trauma. The fear of losing everything or the constant worry about not having enough money can consume individuals and create a pervasive sense of insecurity and vulnerability.

What is the spiritual meaning of losing money? ›

While this is by no means meant as any form of financial advise, I think 'losing money' from a spiritual and/or symbolic viewpoint could mean that you need to learn how to let go and cease attachment to things in life. In dreams, money is often seen as a symbol of power.

What is the psychological consequence of money? ›

These are some common ways money can affect your mental health: Certain situations might trigger feelings of anxiety and panic, like opening envelopes or attending a benefits assessment. Worrying about money can lead to sleep problems. You might not be able to afford the things you need to stay well.

How much does money affect mental health? ›

Financial challenges are associated with mental health challenges. People with the lowest incomes in a given area are 1.5 to 3 times as likely to experience mental health issues, like depression and anxiety, as high-income people in the same area.

What part of the brain controls money? ›

There are two primary parts of the brain involved when we make financial decisions: the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex. The amygdala lies within the limbic system, and is often called the reflexive brain, or reptilian or lizard brain.

What happens when your body is in too much pain? ›

Typically, people experiencing acute pain will have an elevated heart rate, blood pressure and respiratory rate; they may shake or shiver, have goose bumps and pale skin. The more intense the pain, the more visible these signs and symptoms are.

What is the best painkiller for neuropathy? ›

Tramadol. Tramadol is a powerful painkiller related to morphine that can be used to treat neuropathic pain that does not respond to other treatments a GP can prescribe. Like all opioids, tramadol can be addictive if it's taken for a long time. It'll usually only be prescribed for a short time.

What is one of the most debilitating forms of nerve pain? ›

One of the classic examples of neuropathic pain is Tic douloureux. Without treatment, this is a debilitating disorder that involves attacks of severe pain in the facial area (also referred to as trigeminal neuralgia).

What does constant pain do to the brain? ›

“The study shows people with chronic pain experience disruptions in the communication between brain cells. This could lead to a change in personality through a reduction of their ability to effectively process emotions.

When chronic pain becomes too much? ›

If you are affected by severe chronic pain, you should see a pain management specialist. These professionals have specialized expertise in managing symptoms caused by chronic and painful conditions.

How to block pain receptors? ›

The following techniques can help you take your mind off the pain and may help to override established pain signals.
  1. Deep breathing. ...
  2. Eliciting the relaxation response. ...
  3. Meditation with guided imagery. ...
  4. Mindfulness. ...
  5. Yoga and tai chi. ...
  6. Positive thinking.
Apr 11, 2015

How do I get over the pain of losing money? ›

7 Ways to Cope With a Financial Loss
  1. Do not take any impulsive action. ...
  2. Consider taking professional help for emotional support. ...
  3. Assess the situation impartially. ...
  4. Cut back on your expenses for some time. ...
  5. Increase sources of income. ...
  6. Take measures to avoid similar losses in future. ...
  7. Take a Personal Loan.
Sep 5, 2022

Why do I feel bad when I lose money? ›

The abrupt alteration of your Life Script, changes in your lifestyle and housing, and shattering of dreams for yourself and your family all magnify the emotions that surround financial loss.

How to cope with losing a lot of money? ›

Try to take your emotions out of the picture when you're assessing your situation and looking for solutions. Instead of trying to make yourself feel better about the loss, get comfortable accepting the loss. Stay focused on practical steps such as earning extra income or reducing costs.

How do you deal with guilt of losing money? ›

It's vital that you identify the beliefs underlying your guilt and change them. The first question you must ask, though, is whether your guilt is warranted. Guilt can protect us. It isn't a good idea to rack up so many losses that you can't pay it back easily.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: The Hon. Margery Christiansen

Last Updated:

Views: 6176

Rating: 5 / 5 (50 voted)

Reviews: 81% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: The Hon. Margery Christiansen

Birthday: 2000-07-07

Address: 5050 Breitenberg Knoll, New Robert, MI 45409

Phone: +2556892639372

Job: Investor Mining Engineer

Hobby: Sketching, Cosplaying, Glassblowing, Genealogy, Crocheting, Archery, Skateboarding

Introduction: My name is The Hon. Margery Christiansen, I am a bright, adorable, precious, inexpensive, gorgeous, comfortable, happy person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.